Internal and External Validity
General Issues
Developed by: W. Huitt, J. Hummel, D. Kaeck
Last revised: January, 1999
Internal Validity
One of the keys to understanding internal validity (IV) is the recognition that
when it is associated with experimental research it refers both to how well the study was
run (research design, operational definitions used, how variables were measured, what
was/wasn't measured, etc.), and how confidently one can conclude that the change in the
dependent variable was produced solely by the independent variable and not extraneous
ones. In group experimental research, IV answers the question, "Was it really the
treatment that caused the difference between the means/variances of the subjects in the
control and experimental groups?" Similarily, in single-subject research (e.g., ABAB
or multiple baseline), IV attempts to answer the question, "Do I really believe that
it was my treatment that caused a change in the subject's behavior, or could it have been
a result of some other factor?" In descriptive studies (correlational, etc.) internal
validity refers only to the accuracy/quality of the study (e.g., how well the study was
run-see beginning of this paragraph).
In their classic book on experimental research, Campbell and Stanley (1966) identify
and discuss 8 types of extraneous variables that can, if not controlled, jeopardize an
experiment's internal validity.
- History--These are the unique experiences subjects have between the various
measurements done in an experiment. These experiences function like extra, and unplanned,
independent variables. Compounding this, the experiences are likely to vary across
subjects which has a differential effect on the subjects' responses. Studies that take
repeated measures on subjects over time are more likely to be affected by history
variables than those that collect data in shorter time periods, or that do not use
repeated measures.
- Maturation--These are natural (rather than experimenter imposed) changes that
occur as a result of the normal passage of time. For example, the more time that passes in
a study the more likely subjects are to become tired and bored, more or less motivated as
a function of hunger or thirst, older, etc. As Isaac and Michael (1971) point out,
subjects may perform better or worse on a dependent variable not as a result of the
independent variable but because they are older, more/less motivated, etc.
- Testing--Many experiments pretest subjects to establish that all the subjects are
starting the study at approximately the same level, etc. A consequence of pretesting
programs/protocols is that they can contaminate/change the subjects' performance on later
tests (e.g., those used as dependent variables) that measure the same domain beyond any
effects caused by the treatment itself.
- Instrumentation--Changing the measurement methods (or their administration)
during a study affects what is measured. Additionally, if human observers are used, it may
be the judgment of the observer(s) that change over time rather than the subjects'
performance.
- Statistical Regression--When subjects in a study are selected as participants
because they scored extremely high or extremely low on some measure of performance (e.g.,
a test, etc.), retesting of the subjects will almost always produce a different
distribution of scores, and the average for this new distribution will be closer to the
population's. For example, if the chosen subjects all had high scores initially, the
group's average on the retest will be lower (i.e, less extreme) than it was originally.
Conversely, if the group's mean was originally low, their retest mean would be higher.
- Selection--The subjects in comparison (e.g., the control and experimental) groups
should be functionally equivalent at the beginning of a study. If they are, then observed
differences between the groups, as measured by the performance dependent variable(s), at
the end of the study are more likely to be caused only by the independent variable instead
of organismic ones. If the comparison groups are different from one another at the
beginning of the study the results of the study are biased.
- Experimental Mortality--Subjects drop out of studies. If one comparison group
experiences a higher level of subject withdrawal/mortality than other groups, then
observed differences between groups become questionable. Were the observed differences
produced by the independent variable or by the different drop out rates? (Mortality is
also a threat when drop out rates are similar across comparison groups but high.)
- Selection Interactions--In some studies the selection method interacts with one
or more of the other threats (described above), biasing the study's results.
External Validity
The extent to which a study's results (regardless of whether the study is descriptive
or experimental) can be generalized/applied to other people or settings reflects its external
validity. Typically, group research employing randomization will initially possess
higher external validity than will studies (e.g., case studies and single-subject
experimental research) that do not use random selection/assignment. Campbell and Stanley
(cited in Isaac & Michael, 1971) have identified 4 factors that adversely affect a
study's external validity.
- An interaction between how the subjects were selected and the treatment (e.g.,
the independent variable) can occur. If subjects are not randomly selected from a
population, then their particular demographic/organismic characteristics may bias their
performance and the study's results may not be applicable to the population or to another
group that more accurately represents the characteristics of the population.
- Pretesting subjects in a study may cause them to react more/less strongly to the
treatment than they would have had they not experienced the pretest. In such situations
the researcher(s) cannot conclude that members of the population who were not pretested
would perform in a similar manner to the participants in the study. Restated, to
generalize the results of the study the researcher would have to specify that a particular
type of pretesting also be done because the pretesting could be serving as an extra,
unintentional independent variable.
- The performance of subjects in some studies is more a product or reaction to the
experimental setting (e.g., the situation where the study is conducted) than it is
to the independent variable. For example, subjects who know they are participants in a
study, or who are aware of being observed, etc., may react differently to the treatment
than a subject who experienced the treatment but was not aware of being observed, etc.
- Studies that use multiple treatments/interventions may have limited
generalizability because the early treatments may have a cumulative effect on the
subjects' performance. If a group experienced treatment X1, and the first treatment was
followed by a second (X2), their measured performance after X2 will be affected by both
treatments not just X2's because the effects of X1 are not erasable.
Increasing Internal and External Validity
In group research, the primary methods used to achieve internal and external validity
are randomization, the use of a research design and statistical analysis that are
appropriate to the types of data collected, and the question(s) the investigator(s) is
trying to answer. Single-subject experimental studies almost always have high internal
validity because subjects serve as their own controls but, as mentioned earlier, are
extremely low with respect to external validity. Single-subject studies acquire external
validity through the process of replication and extension (i.e., repeating the study in
different settings, with a different subject, etc.). The results of group studies are also
more acceptable by the scientific community when replicated.
References
- Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimental and quasi-experimental
designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally.
- Isaac, S., & Michael, W. B. (1971). Handbook in research and evaluation. San Diego:
EdITS.
Return to:
All materials on this website
[http://www.edpsycinteractive.org] are, unless otherwise stated, the property of
William G. Huitt. Copyright and other intellectual property laws protect these
materials. Reproduction or retransmission of the materials, in whole or in part,
in any manner, without the prior written consent of the copyright holder, is a
violation of copyright law.